When
considering alternate designs for your final evaluation – experimental
or quasi-experimental – you will need to understand the four basic
building blocks: Treatment, People, Observations and Time. In this
section, these elements are first described, followed by the
presentation of standard ‘equations’ that succinctly capture how they
can be combined in particular ways to form basic designs.
1.
People
(N=nonrandom; R=random; C=cut score-differentiate
expert/novice) participating in the research. This can include
individuals or existing groups of people who will participate in the
various treatments for comparison. These individuals or groups will
experience a treatment (one form of training or another).
2.
Observations
(O) or measures being influenced by the technology. The set of
one or more measures is often simply written as O, but if multiple
measures are being assessed the individual measures may be identified
through subscript values, such as O1 for skill, O2
for knowledge, O3 for confidence, O4 for
efficiency, O5 for affect, etc. There are different methods
for conducting measurements (e.g. performance or self-report survey).
If there is variation in these methods being used this notation could be
extended with a second subscript value (e.g. O1,1).
3.
Treatment (X)
or intervention of interest (e.g., training). The simulation or training
technology being evaluated is considered a treatment. More than one form
of the experimental training would be denoted X1, X2,…
Xn. Control groups can be denoted X0.
4.
Time
between the training involving the new technology and the subsequent
measurement of outcomes. For instance, a simple pre/post treatment, by
definition, involves measurement before and after a treatment to support
calculating the change that treatment may have made. More complex
designs could involve even more measures over time or a sequence of
treatments and measurement over time.
Experimental design equations consist of one row per treatment, with the
elements on each row specified in the order in which those actions would
be taken. For instance, the following represents a pre-post test
randomized control group design where one experimental group treatment
is being compared to the control group treatment:
R O
X0 O
R O
X1 O
pre-post test randomized control group
design
The four columns represent four steps:
1.
participants are randomly (R) assigned to
either the experimental group (X1) or the control group (X0)
2.
the same pretests (O) are administered to
both groups
3.
participants in X1 are given the
experimental treatment (e.g. trained using the new technology), while
participants in X0 are given the control treatment (e.g.
trained using an existing training approach)
4.
upon completion of training, participants in
both groups are administered post tests (O).
These way in which these key
components are put together gives rise to additional design concepts and
considerations which are important in selecting alternative research
designs.
·
Pre-Post Test Designs
-
Pretest-posttest designs are the preferred method to compare participant
groups and measure the degree of change occurring as a result of
treatments or interventions.
·
Post test only designs
- If no pretest
is given, it is difficult to ascertain the degree or amount of change
attributable to the treatment. However,
if the goal is to determine if criterion level of performance can be
obtained, the degree of change is not necessary.
·
Control group designs
- Control
groups are intended to provide a group against which to compare the
treatment group to see examine changes related to the treatment. Groups
are compared in which one group, the control group may have no treatment or they may have some neutral treatment
(e.g., when a placebo is used in a clinical trial).
·
Repeated measures designs
– The repeated
measures design uses the same subjects with every condition of the research,
including the control.
This design can be used with smaller sample sizes, however It is
susceptible to practice or test taking effects, as well as order
effects. The order of the treatment and control conditions must be
counterbalanced across participants (i.e., some participants would get
the experimental treatment first followed by the control while others
received the control condition first followed by the experimental
treatment)
·
Within versus between subjects designs –
in within
subject designs, ever participant is receives every condition; while
between subject designs maintain independence. Subjects only participate
in one condition. In general within subjects designs allow you to use
smaller sample sizes, but you must be wary of testing or carryover
effects from repeated measurement. Between subjects designs require more
participants and avoid potential problems associated with carryover
effects.
·
Randomized versus non-equivalent groups
– Randomization is the preferred approach. The goal of random assignment
is to ensure equivalence of the two (or more) groups at the start of the
experiment. This helps us make inferences that any changes in the post
training measures are due to the training and not to any differences
that may have existed prior to the treatment (e.g., that one group
already had higher levels of knowledge / skill before the experimental
training occurred). In field experiments, random assignment may not
always be easy or possible. If participants cannot be randomly assigned
to groups, a non-equivalent group design can be used. Other design
concepts such as pre-post testing can help you address potential
differences that may exist prior to the treatment.
·
Randomized Block designs
- If specific
differences among groups of subjects exist a randomized
block design may
be appropriate. Blocking is a strategy for grouping people in your data
analysis in order to reduce noise -- it is an analysis
strategy. In a block design, data for experimental subjects are
divided into homogeneous blocks (e.g., based on different locations). Blocking
doesn't necessarily affect anything that you do with the research
participants. |